Anunţ publicitar al Statului Român in ziarele mari ale lumii:

Anunţ publicitar al Statului Român in ziarele mari ale lumii:

Cine a putut, ştiut şi vrut a plecat.

Avem nevoie de ajutor!
Plătim la nivelul pieţei.
Preferăm vorbitori de Româna!

______________________________

marți, mai 31

romanilor plecati, dar nu numai

iata de ce trebuie sa va invatati copiii (si) limba romana, oriunde sunteti:

A cognitive neuroscientist, Ellen Bialystok has spent almost 40 years
learning about how bilingualism sharpens the mind. Her good news:
Among other benefits, the regular use of two languages appears to
delay the onset of Alzheimer's disease symptoms. Dr. Bialystok, 62, a
distinguished research professor of psychology at York University in
Toronto, was awarded a $100,000 Killam Prize last year for her
contributions to social science. We spoke for two hours in a
Washington hotel room in February and again, more recently, by
telephone. An edited version of the two conversations follows.

Q. How did you begin studying bilingualism?

A. You know, I didn't start trying to find out whether bilingualism
was bad or good. I did my doctorate in psychology: on how children
acquire language. When I finished graduate school, in 1976, there was
a job shortage in Canada for Ph.D.'s. The only position I found was
with a research project studying second language acquisition in school
children. It wasn't my area. But it was close enough.

As a psychologist, I brought neuroscience questions to the study, like
"How does the acquisition of a second language change thought?" It was
these types of questions that naturally led to the bilingualism
research. The way research works is, it takes you down a road. You
then follow that road.

Q. So what exactly did you find on this unexpected road?

A. As we did our research, you could see there was a big difference in
the way monolingual and bilingual children processed language. We
found that if you gave 5- and 6-year-olds language problems to solve,
monolingual and bilingual children knew, pretty much, the same amount
of language.

But on one question, there was a difference. We asked all the children
if a certain illogical sentence was grammatically correct: "Apples
grow on noses." The monolingual children couldn't answer. They'd say,
"That's silly" and they'd stall. But the bilingual children would say,
in their own words, "It's silly, but it's grammatically correct." The
bilinguals, we found, manifested a cognitive system with the ability
to attend to important information and ignore the less important.

Q. How does this work — do you understand it?

A. Yes. There's a system in your brain, the executive control system.
It's a general manager. Its job is to keep you focused on what is
relevant, while ignoring distractions. It's what makes it possible for
you to hold two different things in your mind at one time and switch
between them.

If you have two languages and you use them regularly, the way the
brain's networks work is that every time you speak, both languages pop
up and the executive control system has to sort through everything and
attend to what's relevant in the moment. Therefore the bilinguals use
that system more, and it's that regular use that makes that system
more efficient.

Q. One of your most startling recent findings is that bilingualism
helps forestall the symptoms of Alzheimer's disease. How did you come
to learn this?

A. We did two kinds of studies. In the first, published in 2004, we
found that normally aging bilinguals had better cognitive functioning
than normally aging monolinguals. Bilingual older adults performed
better than monolingual older adults on executive control tasks. That
was very impressive because it didn't have to be that way. It could
have turned out that everybody just lost function equally as they got
older.

That evidence made us look at people who didn't have normal cognitive
function. In our next studies , we looked at the medical records of
400 Alzheimer's patients. On average, the bilinguals showed
Alzheimer's symptoms five or six years later than those who spoke only
one language. This didn't mean that the bilinguals didn't have
Alzheimer's. It meant that as the disease took root in their brains,
they were able to continue functioning at a higher level. They could
cope with the disease for longer.

Q. So high school French is useful for something other than ordering a
special meal in a restaurant?

A. Sorry, no. You have to use both languages all the time. You won't
get the bilingual benefit from occasional use.

Q. One would think bilingualism might help with multitasking — does it?

A. Yes, multitasking is one of the things the executive control system
handles. We wondered, "Are bilinguals better at multitasking?" So we
put monolinguals and bilinguals into a driving simulator. Through
headphones, we gave them extra tasks to do — as if they were driving
and talking on cellphones. We then measured how much worse their
driving got. Now, everybody's driving got worse. But the bilinguals,
their driving didn't drop as much. Because adding on another task
while trying to concentrate on a driving problem, that's what
bilingualism gives you — though I wouldn't advise doing this.

Q. Has the development of new neuroimaging technologies changed your work?

A. Tremendously. It used to be that we could only see what parts of
the brain lit up when our subjects performed different tasks. Now,
with the new technologies, we can see how all the brain structures
work in accord with each other.

In terms of monolinguals and bilinguals, the big thing that we have
found is that the connections are different. So we have monolinguals
solving a problem, and they use X systems, but when bilinguals solve
the same problem, they use others. One of the things we've seen is
that on certain kinds of even nonverbal tests, bilingual people are
faster. Why? Well, when we look in their brains through neuroimaging,
it appears like they're using a different kind of a network that might
include language centers to solve a completely nonverbal problem.
Their whole brain appears to rewire because of bilingualism.

Q. Bilingualism used to be considered a negative thing — at least in
the United States. Is it still?

A. Until about the 1960s, the conventional wisdom was that
bilingualism was a disadvantage. Some of this was xenophobia. Thanks
to science, we now know that the opposite is true.

Q. Many immigrants choose not to teach their children their native
language. Is this a good thing?

A. I'm asked about this all the time. People e-mail me and say, "I'm
getting married to someone from another culture, what should we do
with the children?" I always say, "You're sitting on a potential
gift."

There are two major reasons people should pass their heritage language
onto children. First, it connects children to their ancestors. The
second is my research: Bilingualism is good for you. It makes brains
stronger. It is brain exercise.

Q. Are you bilingual?

A. Well, I have fully bilingual grandchildren because my daughter
married a Frenchman. When my daughter announced her engagement to her
French boyfriend, we were a little surprised. It's always astonishing
when your child announces she's getting married. She said, "But Mom,
it'll be fine, our children will be bilingual!"

Niciun comentariu: